Research Article |
Corresponding author: Neil D'Cruze ( neildcruze@worldanimalprotection.org ) Academic editor: Krizler Tanalgo
© 2024 Neil D'Cruze, Angie Elwin, Shubhobroto Ghosh, Alexander E. Asfaw, Emma Coulthard, David Megson, John Norrey, Sangita Giri, Vasudha Mishra, Tiasa Adhya, Suvrajyoti Chatterjee, Meghna Banerjee, Aditya Banerjee, Lauren Harrington.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
D’Cruze N, Elwin A, Ghosh S, Asfaw AE, Coulthard E, Megson D, Norrey J, Giri S, Mishra V, Adhya T, Chatterjee S, Banerjee M, Banerjee A, Harrington L (2024) Ritualistic hunts: exploring the motivations and conservation implications in West Bengal, India. Nature Conservation 56: 243-273. https://doi.org/10.3897/natureconservation.56.132178
|
Ritualistic hunts are illegal, large, organised cultural events which are a prevalent concern in West Bengal from both an animal welfare and conservation perspective. We carried out a socio-economic survey with 112 individuals in the districts of Jhargram and West Medinipur to better understand the characteristics of these hunts, the species impacted, and the drivers and motivations of the communities that engage in these types of illegal activity. Specifically, we asked which wild animals were most desirable, which were most profitable, what derivatives from hunted animals were used for, and which wild animals were perceived to have increased most in rarity. We found that these events involve both indiscriminate and targeted killing of a wide variety of wildlife, including at least 93 inferred species (seven of which are categorised on the IUCN Red List as threatened (i.e. either Vulnerable, Endangered or Critically Endangered); in 34 the population trend has been categorised as declining, and 25 are listed on Schedule I of India’s Wildlife Protection Act). We found that wild meat/food was the most frequently stated personal use of hunted wildlife in addition to belief-based use, traditional medicine, and decorative use. Engagement in the onward commercial sale of wildlife was also reported by more than a third of hunters. However, personal enjoyment was identified by most hunters as their main motivation for taking part in ritualistic hunts. Despite widespread engagement in ritualistic hunting, we found that the majority of hunters also expressed a willingness to engage in legal non-consumptive alternatives if they were made available. As such, we recommend that in addition to effective law enforcement, further research to identify viable non-consumptive alternatives and inform associated human behaviour change initiatives could help deliver a positive transformation for both wildlife and people in West Bengal.
Animal welfare, illegal wildlife trade, protected species, Shikar Utsav, traditional hunting
Hunting is a threat to wildlife across the tropics (
Ritualistic hunts typically involve large numbers of people who engage in group hunting of various wildlife species on certain days of the year, usually coinciding with a day of cultural or religious significance (
Hunting has been illegal in India since 1972 when the Wildlife (Protection) Act (WPA) was originally enacted. Updated in 2022, the WPA remains a comprehensive piece of legislation, under which hunting of all forms of wildlife in India is prohibited, as is the possession and trade in wild animal derivatives (including trophies) without previous permission in writing from the Chief Wildlife Warden or the Authorized Officer (Legislative Department, Ministry of Law and Justice, Government of India). Although traditional community rights to wildlife and forest resources are granted under Section 3(l) of the Scheduled Tribes & Other Traditional Forest Dwellers (Recognition of Forest Rights) Act, 2006 or Forest Rights Act (FRA), the FRA specifically excludes the use of forest animals as a right (i.e., hunting or trapping or extracting a part of the body of any species of wild animal). India has been a signatory to the Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora (CITES) since 1976. CITES aims to regulate international legal trade to safeguard certain listed species from overexploitation (www.cites.org). Species, such as tigers and pangolins are currently listed in Appendix I of CITES, and as such, international commercial trade in these animals, their parts, or derivatives is essentially prohibited (
In relation to the situation in West Bengal, the continuation of these festivals recently prompted a decision by the Calcutta High Court in early 2023 which equated “indiscriminate wildlife hunting” with murder under Section 302 of the Indian Penal Code (
Despite substantial media coverage in recent years (e.g.
The study was carried out in West Bengal (Fig.
Map showing the location of Jhargram and West Medinipur districts in southern West Bengal, India. Administrative boundaries data source: https://www.diva-gis.org/gdata; mapping software: QGIS (www.qgis.org). The boundaries shown, and the designations used on this map may not be correct and do not imply any official endorsement and/or acceptance by the authors or their respective institutional affiliations.
In both districts, a large proportion (94% and 88%, respectively) of the population live in rural areas in relatively small, closely spaced villages (average distance between neighbouring villages = 1.17 km,
The questionnaire survey was conducted in August and September 2022 by local field staff who asked a set of 26 predetermined questions (Suppl. material
We interviewed participants who self-identified as having engaged in ritualistic hunting (either in the past or present) and were willing to participate in the study, through a process of chain referral (
Participants were initially asked some non-hunt related questions such as what pets they keep before being asked about their involvement in ritualistic hunting in an attempt to create a comfortable and relaxing environment (
Specifically, participants were asked to identify the 10 wild animals (using local common names) that they currently considered to be the most profitable (as opposed to most valuable per item or most commonly traded, with wildlife body parts and live animals considered separately), the 10 wild animals that they considered to be the most desirable (i.e. attractive and useful), and the 10 wild animals that they considered to have most increased in rarity (and therefore inferred reduced availability) over the past five years (corresponding to the period 2017–2021). Common names relating to taxonomic class (e.g., bird and mammal) or below were included in the analysis of the survey responses but were excluded when considering conservation status. Participants were also asked to provide additional information including the wildlife body parts sold per unit, price paid per unit, where the body parts were sold (locally / nationally / internationally), and their intended purpose categorised as “food” (wild meat and other nutritional use), “spiritual” (e.g., items to invoke magic, good luck and prosperity), “medicinal” (ingested or topical treatments for illness), “clothing and decoration” (for fashion and aesthetic purposes), and “unknown” (which included unknown purposes).
Interviews were conducted in Bengali and later translated into English. In accordance with the British Sociological Association Statement of Ethical Practice (
We used descriptive statistics, frequency histograms and pie charts to describe, and to illustrate, patterns in the data. Chi-squared tests of association were used to describe the distribution of age groups and the education level of participants, and to test for differences in perceived trends in the number of animals in the forest, and among rankings for proposed drivers for taking part in hunting rituals. Spearman’s rank correlation was used to assess the relationship between the species that were stated as becoming increasingly rare and the declared most desirable and most profitable species. Word clouds were used to summarise and to illustrate the answers given to open-ended questions where there was sufficient text available (excluding the terms “activity” and “animals”); otherwise, we described the key words used by hunters to describe their actions or feelings. Data descriptions and statistical analyses were carried out in R (version 4.2.3;
We interviewed a total of 112 people (hereafter respondents) from 93 villages in two districts of West Bengal: Jhargram (n = 59), and West Medinipur (n = 53). Ninety-nine respondents identified themselves as hunters; 13 respondents self-identified as “non-hunters” but provided information on someone else who engages in hunting. All but one of the respondents were male (one of the non-hunters was female). Respondents ranged in age between 18 and 76, with an approximately even distribution across age groups (grouped as 18–25, 26–35, 36–45, 46–55, 56–65, > 66: χ2 = 2.92, df = 5, p = 0.712) and an average age of about 40 (median = 38.5, mean = 41.6, n = 56; where respondents gave an age range rather than a precise number of years we used the mid-value so the average may be slightly underestimated). Where sample size does not equate to 112, the remainder did not answer the question. Most (n = 81, 72.3%) respondents were married (15 were unmarried and one was widowed). Most reported owning one house (n = 90, 80.4%, two reported owning two houses), and most had lived in the village since birth (n = 98, 87.5%). Others had lived in the village for between 5 and 50 years (n = 7, 6.3%), “since marriage” (n = 1, 0.9%), or described themselves as “resident” (n = 3, 2.7%) or “migrant” (n = 3, 2.7%) but did not say how long they had lived there. Households reportedly comprised between two and 14 people (mean = 5.9, n = 78) and included between one and three children, mostly (n = 20) two children (mean = 1.8, n = 39). Two (1.8%) respondents reported, respectively, “10–15” and “12–20” people in the household.
The education level of respondents varied and was split relatively evenly amongst those that had reportedly not received any formal education (n = 20, 17.9%), educated to primary level (n = 27, 24.1%, included one who was only “nursery” educated), or to secondary level (n = 17, 15.2%, including 9 at “secondary” level and 8 at “higher secondary” level), but only five (4.5%) respondents were educated to college level (χ2 = 9.10, df = 3, p = 0.028, education level grouped as “no formal education”, “primary”, “secondary” and “college”). When asked for their ethnicity, most respondents (n = 72) reported only that they were “Adivasi” (a term usually used to refer to Scheduled Tribes in official records,
When asked about how frequently they had been involved in ritualistic hunting (hereafter ‘hunting’/ ‘hunts’) in the last 12 months, the majority (82.7%) of respondents who answered the question (n = 75) said that they took part in hunts only once or twice (“once”: n = 55, “1–2 times”: n = 7). A small number of hunters (n = 10) reported that they were involved in more frequent hunts: seven took part in hunts between two and five times in the last 12 months, and three reported taking part, respectively, seven times, 13–14 times, and “many” times. Three respondents reported that they had not taken part in a hunt in the last 12 months (although two of these confirmed that they had taken part in hunts 2–3 or 3–4 times in the last 5 years); 24 did not answer the question. Accordingly, the majority of hunters reported taking part in between one and 15 hunts over the last five years, most (n = 48, 77.4% of the 62 respondents who gave numeric answers to this question) stated five or fewer; two respondents reported taking part in, respectively, 25 hunts, and 35–40 hunts.
All but one of the survey respondents (who did not answer this particular question), described “thousands” of people collectively taking part in a ritual hunt, in hunting parties of 20–40 people (overall minimum 10, maximum 200 or 300), using spears (Ballam/ Kencha; n = 87), bows and arrows (Kaar baansh/ Teer dhonukh; n = 71), catapults (Batul/ Gulti; n = 56) and a range of other methods including hand axes (Tangi) and wooden sticks (Lathi; Fig.
A pie chart showing proportion of hunters using different hunting/killing methods during ritualistic hunting in West Bengal. In most cases, hunters reported using more than one method (five hunters said they only used spears, and one only used snares) and so the number of methods exceeds the number of survey respondents (n = 99 hunters, n = 252 methods reportedly used; 10 survey respondents did not say which methods they used) B bar chart showing the months reported to be best for hunting. Where hunters suggested a range of months, we counted each month within the specified range, and so the total frequency of months being specified as ‘optimal’ exceeds the number of survey respondents (n = 99 hunters, n = total frequency of months specified; 13 survey respondents did not answer the question) C pie chart showing perceived trends in the number of animals in the forest compared with 5 years ago (based on the answers of 75 hunters), five-point scale: a lot less / quite a few less / the same / quite a lot more / a lot more.
The best time for hunting was most commonly reported to be between March and May (n = 73 respondents suggested one, or a range of months, within this period), although some hunters suggested that hunting could start as early as January or February, and others suggested that it could take place later in the year (starting in April/May and extending to June/July) (Fig.
Estimates of the number of animals captured and killed, by the hunting party, in the last 12 months, varied between 1–2 and, in most cases, up to 20 (with most respondents answering towards the lower end of this range: mean = 4.1, median = 2.5, n = 61, excluding one outlier); three respondents reported that the hunting group captured and killed 20–25, 25–30, and 100–200 animals, respectively. Accordingly, estimates of the number of animals captured and killed by the hunting party over the last five years were predominantly (n = 43) between 10 and 50; 11 respondents estimated that the hunting party had captured and killed fewer than 10 animals in the last five years. Six hunters suggested that the numbers captured and killed over the last five years exceeded 50: four estimated 50–100, one > 100, and one 1,000–2,000 (one hunter reported that they had “lost count”).
Fifty-four survey respondents stated specifically that the animals captured are “slaughtered at home” (some that they are taken to the “leader’s” house where they are slaughtered and divided), 32 described “slaughtering” and cooking animals in the forest (three of these also said that the animals were eaten in the forest). Two respondents explained that if they are hunting far away, and stay overnight in the forest, the animals are slaughtered there but otherwise they bring them home, and six refer to slaughtering animals at home or in the forest (three also said that the “excess” is brought home). Respondents referred to transporting the animals killed by carrying them on their shoulders (in some cases with their feet tied to sticks), on bikes, or in jeeps or other 4-wheelers.
Overall, during our survey, the 99 respondents who self-identified as hunters used 74 different local names to refer to 53 distinct different common names of the most desirable and profitable wildlife species that they currently hunt (plus at least four unidentified animals), comprising 36 common names referring to birds (67.9%), 11 to mammals (20.8%), five to reptiles (9.4%), and one insect (ants) (Suppl. material
A total of 52 unique common names were given when participants were asked to list the ‘ten most desirable wildlife species or body part from ritualistic hunting’. All respondents (n = 99) provided answers, and respondents each listed 2–15 species. The total number of species mentioned was highest for birds (n = 35 species) followed by mammals (n = 11), reptiles (n = 5) and insects (ants, n = 1). The most frequently mentioned common names (scientific names are inferred) for the ‘ten most desirable wildlife species hunted’ were wild boar (Sus scrofa, n = 99 mentions, 100% of hunters), followed by Indian hare (Lepus nigricollis, n = 96, 97.0%), greater coucal (Centropus sinensis, n = 64, 64.6%), quail (Phasianidae, n = 53, 53.5%), collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto, n = 47, 47.5%), yellow-footed green pigeon (Treron phoenicopterus, n = 47, 47.5%), jungle cat (Felis chaus, n = 41, 41.4%) and Bengal monitor (Varanus bengalensis, n = 22, 22.2%) (Fig.
Breakdown of most desirable species by body part and purpose. Frequency refers to the number of times a respondent reported each purpose of use of desirable species’ body parts. Note: the responses were categorised as “clothing and decoration”, “food”, “medicinal”, and “spiritual” based on their inferred meaning (see text for full description of items within each category). The full list of common names provided along with local and inferred scientific names is given in Suppl. material
A total of eight different body parts related to the 52 most desirable species that were mentioned at least once, with ‘whole body’ being most frequently cited (94% of a total of 757 species and body part combinations mentioned. Note: each respondent listed up to 15 most desirable species) (Fig.
Only six respondents provided sale prices for the most desirable species; the most expensive items were reportedly derived from red junglefowl (Gallus gallus, Rs 2200–2300 (~26–27 USD) per whole animal for consumption) and Indian hare (Rs 500 (~6 USD) per animal for consumption) (Suppl. material
Sixteen unique common names (6 birds, 5 mammals, and 5 reptiles) were given when participants were asked to list the ‘ten most profitable wildlife species or body part from ritualistic hunting’ (n = 58 provided answers, and respondents each listed 1–5 species). The most frequently mentioned common names were wild boar (n = 53, 91.4%), followed by Indian flying fox (n = 13, 22.4%), Bengal monitor (n = 8, 13.8%), Russell’s viper (n = 6, 10.3%), and elongated tortoise (Indotestudo elongata, n = 6, 10.3%) (Fig.
Breakdown of most profitable species hunted by body part and purpose. Frequency refers to the number of times a respondent reported each purpose of use of profitable species’ body parts. Note: categories were inferred during analysis (“Unknown/Other” includes unknown purposes (n = 21) and wild boar “mouth parts” used to “stop children crying” (n = 6); see text for full description of items within each category).
Eleven different body parts related to the 16 most profitable species mentioned with ‘bones’ being most frequently cited (n = 43, 27% of a total of 159 species and body part combinations mentioned), along with ‘fat/oil’ (n = 26, 16.4%), and ‘head/head parts’ (n = 14, 8.8%) (Fig.
Seventeen respondents provided prices for the most profitable species; the most expensive individual items were derived from the Bengal monitor (Rs 3000–4000 (~36–48 USD) per whole animal skin), Indian hare (Rs 700–800 (~8.4–9.6 USD) per kg or Rs 500 (~6 USD) per animal for consumption), and Russell’s viper (Rs 500–600 (~6–7.2 USD) per ‘piece’ of bones) (Suppl. material
When asked specifically if they hunted for personal use, while all 99 hunters interviewed reported that they used the animals hunted for personal ‘consumption’ (i.e. food), 41 of these also referred to “other uses”. Other uses were most often associated with wild boar – specifically, the fat oil (used for “massage for joint pains”, cooking, or traditional medicine; n = 18), the teeth, tusks, or “head parts” (to ward off “bad omens”, “evil spirits” or to avoid “bad luck”; n = 33, one also said that they kept the head as a souvenir after they had sold the body for meat), the fur (used as a brush for a bicycle; n = 11), or the bones (used for medicine; n = 1). Respondents also mentioned using the bones of Indian flying fox, birds, owls, or Russell’s vipers, primarily to “ward off bad omens” or “evil spirits”, or (occasionally) for medicine (n = 14), tortoiseshell to treat pain (n = 1) or the skins of Bengal monitors, Indian rat snakes, or golden jackals for unknown purposes (n = 3). Thirteen respondents in this set of questions referred to “other people” using these body parts, or that they were “not sure of the purpose” suggesting that they did not necessarily do these things themselves and were not particularly familiar with these types of use. Three respondents mentioned the personal use of giving wild boar teeth or “mouth parts” to small children (to stop them crying or eating mud).
When asked specifically if they hunted for commercial use, approximately a quarter (n = 27, 27.3%) of respondents said that they sold meat from the hunt at local markets, and 17 said that they sold the body parts “locally”. Meat and body parts were reportedly sold for local consumption (one said that they sometimes gave the meat to relatives in different districts). Of the 27 respondents that sold the meat, 24 specifically stated that the meat was sold only when there was excess available. Hunters reportedly received between Rs 150 and 200 (~1.8–2.4 USD) per kilogram of meat sold (either ‘meat’ of an unspecified species or ‘wild boar’); two hunters suggested as much as Rs 300 (~3.6 USD) per kg for wild boar meat, one hunter suggested Rs 150 (~1.8 USD) per kg for golden jackal meat, and two suggested, respectively between Rs 250 and 300 (~3–3.6 USD) for a “rabbit”, or Rs 500 (~6 USD) for a whole Indian hare. One hunter suggested that francolin and quail were also sold live for Rs 200–300 (~2.4–3.6 USD). One respondent reported that Indian rat snake meat was eaten (but no price was given).
When asked what the body parts were used for when sold, the answers given were similar to those for personal use: respondents most often referred to the use of wild boar teeth, head, or “mouth parts” (n = 33; often as a “garland” (mala)) to ward off bad omens (or sometimes to “stop children eating mud”), or the use of wild boar oil or fat (n = 19) for joint pain, cooking, or tooth ache. The bones of owls, birds (black kite, slender billed vulture (Gyps tenuirostris)
A pair of wild boar teeth were reportedly sold for Rs 30–40 (~0.4–0.5 USD), a wild boar head for Rs 50–60 (~0.6–0.7 USD), wild boar fur for Rs 100 - 200 (~1.2–2.4 USD) (per 250 g), a whole snake for Rs 40–50 (~0.5–0.6 USD) (although the bones of Russell’s viper were reportedly sold in local markets for Rs 50–60 (~0.6–0.7 USD) per snake, Rs 150 Rs (~1.8 USD), or Rs 500–600 (~6–7.2 USD)), and Rs 200 (~2.4 USD) for a pair of birds’ legs. One hunter reported that golden jackal skins sold in local markets for Rs 400–500 (~4.8–6 USD). The highest prices reported were Rs 2200–2300 (~26.5–27.8 USD) per bird for a junglefowl and Rs 3000–4000 (~36–48 USD) for a monitor lizard.
Hunters’ perceptions of the trend in the number of animals in the forest compared with 5 years ago differed significantly from what might be expected under an equal random distribution (χ2 = 38.71, df = 4, p < 0.001): of the 75 hunters that answered this question, 51 (68%) believed that there were fewer animals in the forest than there were 5 years ago (n = 49 that there were “quite a few less”, and 2 that there were “a lot less”, Fig.
When asked which (if any) wild animals had become rare and/or unavailable in the last 5 years, respondents (n = 50) collectively listed 10 species that they believed had become extremely rare (including tigers (Panthera t. tigris), sloth bears (Melursus ursinus), and vultures), 21 that they believed had become slightly rare (mostly bird species, but also golden jackals, jungle cats, and wild boar), and four that were reported to be either extremely or slightly rare by different interviewees (deer [Cervidae], Indian wolf (Canis lupus pallipes), barred buttonquail (Turnix suscitator), and peacock (Pavo cristatus)) (Suppl. material
The most frequently mentioned common names for the ‘ten species which have become rare or unavailable in the last five years’ were ‘deer’ (n = 11, 12.8% of a total of 86 mentions), followed by ‘slender billed vulture’ (which may or may not have been misidentified, see footnote
Only 24 respondents suggested reasons why animals had become rare – of these, 11 (45.8%) suggested that it was due to hunting and 8 (33.3%) suggested it was due to deforestation or burning the forest (sometimes by hunting parties). Other suggested reasons included: irregular rainfall, disease, animals migrating to different areas, and the ignorance of the forest department. Two respondents reported that the number of animals/birds had increased, one due to hunting, and one because people were not able to enter the forest due to elephants.
With regards to international conservation status, three of the 93 inferred hunted species (3.2%) are currently categorised on the International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) Red List as Critically Endangered (elongated tortoise, long billed vulture, and slender billed vulture [see footnote
Breakdown of A IUCN Red List status B population trend, and C Indian Wildlife Protection Act 1972 Schedule status for the species listed as either the most desirable or most profitable species hunted.
With regards to their population trends, 10 (10.8%) inferred species have wild populations considered to be increasing, 29 (31.2%) have wild populations that are stable, 34 (36.6%) that are decreasing, 19 (20.4%) have an unknown population status, and for one sub-species (Indian wolf) the population trend has not been assessed (Fig.
When asked how they felt about ritualistic hunting, all but one of the hunters reported that they strongly liked it (one exception amongst the hunters said that they quite disliked hunting). The 13 non-hunters also reported that they ‘strongly liked’ ritualistic hunting when asked how they felt about the practice. The seven potential drivers for taking part in hunting rituals presented to hunters differed in their reported importance with ‘Fun’ and to a lesser extent ‘Maintaining a traditional practice’ statistically significantly more often highly ranked (ranks 4 or 5; ranked on a scale of 1–5, where 1 is not at all important and 5 is very important) than low or unranked (fun: χ2 = 50.72, df = 1, p < 0.001; traditional practice: χ2 = 11.88, df = 1, p < 0.001, ranks 4 and 5 combined and ranks 3, 2, 1 and unranked combined, tested against an even distribution of responses between “important” and “unimportant” ranks, Fig.
A importance of eight potential drivers for taking part in ritualistic hunts. Drivers were proposed by interviewers and ranked by respondents on a scale of 1–5, where 5 was “very important” and 1 was “not at all important”. Data given are the number of respondents giving a particular rank (we assume that a hunter that did not rank a particular driver – categorised here as “unranked” – did not consider it to be important) B wordcloud showing words used to describe why hunters felt the way they did regarding benefits from hunting to themselves or to the forest. (NB. “paves” was used in the context of “paves the way for…”) (based on responses from 66 hunters).
Of the 99 hunters, 87 (87.9%) said that they did not currently engage in animal-friendly alternatives to ritualistic hunting. All but two of the 88 interviewees that answered this question suggested that they would engage in potential animal-friendly alternatives if they were available, that they would like to see such alternatives, and that awareness programs should be “organised about birds and animals within the forest, and awareness about snakes, which would not involve hunting” (Note that several respondents gave the same responses to these questions using the same words). Responses to this question were the same for the non-hunters: 10 of 13 did not currently engage in alternatives but gave the same answer about awareness programs (3 did not answer the question).
When asked if hunting provided benefits either to themselves or to the forest, 58.6% (n = 58) answered that hunting benefited them, one answered that it benefited the forest (and did not mention personal benefits), four answered that it provided no benefit to either (36 did not answer the question or were not sure). Of the 58 hunters who reported a personal benefit, 56.9% believed that hunting either did not benefit (n = 29) or was harmful (n = 4) to the forest, 27.6% believed that hunting benefited (n = 14) or did not harm (n = 2) the forest (nine did not mention the forest specifically or did not specify a benefit or disbenefit). Personal benefits were attributed to celebration, fun, providing food for their family, and showing off their skills to other hunters, whilst forest benefits were attributed to the numbers of animals in the forest (keeping a “check”, and “pav[ing] the way for younger animals”) and chopping down old trees (Fig.
Hunters confirmed that there are a wide range of species across a number of taxonomic groups that would be considered either desirable or commercially valuable captures during ritualistic hunting festival events in West Bengal (Fig.
A adult and juvenile barking deer killed in Purulia district in 2019 B 2 injured monitor lizards (Varanus salvator) (including a pregnant individual) captured by hunters in Panskura railway station in East Medinipur district in 2018 C hunting weapons, along with hunted wild animals – rose-ringed parakeet, lesser golden-backed woodpecker, white-throated kingfisher, Indian palm squirrel and Indian grey mongoose D mongoose meat being cooked in a vessel on a railway platform. Copyright HEAL.
With regards to scale, hunters also confirmed that hunting festivals are periodic large collective events involving “thousands” of participants that operate in groups that typically range from 20 to 40 (and up to a maximum of 300) people per “hunting party”. This aligns with observations made in other recently published reports. For example,
All 99 of the participants in our survey who self-identified as hunters confirmed that the wild animals captured during ritualistic hunts were used for personal consumption of wild meat as part of celebratory feasts and meat-sharing in the village (
In addition, although most of the species perceived by hunters to be “desirable” were reportedly consumed, some had additional or alternative uses – for example, almost 40% of hunters who listed Bengal monitors as desirable species also used their skins, with personal uses described suggesting that this might have been for belts. More broadly, wild animal parts have historically been sold as jewellery, souvenirs and for medicinal/spiritual purposes by tribal communities in India for millennia (
Beyond the dominant personal consumption of wild meat and the enduring cultural aspects, our analysis of hunter responses regarding the most profitable wildlife species suggests that these ritualistic hunts also comprise a commercial component (albeit for a relative minority of hunters and largely limited to occasions when there was excess meat available). Notably, approximately half of respondents were able to provide information on the profitability of particular species, but not much more than a quarter said that they sold them themselves (i.e. hunters seemed to be aware of the potential value of the species captured and their body parts but, for the most part, did not personally engage in selling them). Moreover, although some hunters also sold wild animal body parts - including wild boar derivatives, Russell’s viper bones, Bengal monitor fat, elongated tortoise shells, Indian rat snake tails, golden jackal skin, and Indian chameleon tails - many were unable to provide information relating to their intended end use. Other recent studies have described cash income as being an important driver for hunting among other tribes in India. For example,
When hunters were questioned more broadly about their motivations for participating in these types of hunting festivals, our study uncovered a consistent positive sentiment toward ritualistic hunts, even among those who identified as non-hunters, despite the illegal status of the hunts (and the fact that many respondents were aware of their illegality). The majority of hunters ranked “engaging in a fun activity” and “maintaining a traditional practice” as important elements of ritualistic hunts, and some (n = 19) also ranked “social cohesion” as important. Personal enjoyment as a primary motivating factor (even above personal use for wild meat) may also help to partly explain recent observations made by a number of conservation-focused NGOs, which have reported that the hunting festivals of south-western West Bengal have recently attracted an influx of hunters from surrounding areas including West Medinipur, Jhargram, Bankura, Murshidabad, Purulia and Jharkhand (
Our research also draws attention to the animal welfare concerns associated with ritualistic hunts. A growing body of research continues to demonstrate the cognitive capacity of wild animals and their ability to experience a range of emotions and states including excitement, fear, frustration, and pain (
Although the wild animals are often slaughtered, skinned, and cooked at the hunting site, they may also be taken away for slaughter elsewhere (
Our study reiterates potential conservation concerns associated with ritualistic hunting in India (e.g.,
Specifically, 28 of the species inferred on the basis of local and common names given by hunters as most desirable or profitable species are not currently listed on the IUCN Red List as threatened but have wild populations that are considered to be either in decline, not assessed or unknown (Suppl. material
Ritualistic hunting in contemporary West Bengal appears to have departed significantly from its original cultural components as described by
Wildlife trade surveys that touch upon illegal and/or unsustainable activities should be interpreted with care; not least because markets are complex systems that can vary over time (due to a variety of different factors) and those involved in the trade chains may distrust surveyors or have poor memory recall (
Rather, our intention was to gain new insights into the diversity of wildlife being targeted (by hunters), to better understand what they might be used for (both by hunters personally and any subsequent consumers following onward sale), and to identify those wildlife species that might potentially be at risk. Likewise, a full inventory of wildlife being hunted in these two districts, including identifying species that are most targeted and the frequency of trade, was beyond the scope of this study. Specifically, we acknowledge that our use of local names to infer the species hunted and sold cannot be considered a complete taxonomic account, and that in some cases hunters may be referring to only one particular species that is not threatened by extinction or vice versa. Yet, despite the limitations of a “snapshot” survey-based approach, we believe that our findings represent valuable information that can be used to help provide information for future efforts to protect wildlife in West Bengal.
Our study highlights the indiscriminate killing of a wide variety of species during illegal ritualistic hunts in West Bengal including potentially at least 25 that are listed on Schedule I of the Wildlife Protection Act. While recognizing that ritualistic hunting is a complex issue involving various stakeholders with diverse perspectives and goals (
This study was funded by an animal welfare organisation; four of the authors are employed by the same organisation (NDC holds the position of Head of Research) and LAH received funding from the same organisation. HEAL is a wildlife conservation organisation. The results presented in this paper were in no way influenced by either the funding source, or our own personal views on animal welfare or conservation.
Ethical approval was obtained via Manchester Metropolitan University on 06/09/2022, EthOS Reference Number: 43711.
This study was funded by an animal welfare organisation.
Conceptualization: Neil D'Cruze, Angie Elwin, Shubhobroto Ghosh, Alexander E. Asfaw; Investigation: Sangita Giri and Suvrajyoti Chatterjee; Data curation: Angie Elwin, Lauren Harrington, Emma Coulthard, John Norrey, Tiasa Adhya and Suvrajyoti Chatterjee; Writing—original draft preparation: Neil D'Cruze, Lauren Harrington, Angie Elwin; Writing—review and editing: Neil D'Cruze, Angie Elwin, Lauren Harrington, Shubhobroto Ghosh, Alexander E. Asfaw, Emma Coulthard, John Norrey, David Megson, Tiasa Adhya, Vasudha Mishra, Meghna Banerjee, Aditya Banerjee; Visualization: Angie Elwin, Lauren Harrington; Supervision: Neil D'Cruze, Shubhobroto Ghosh, David Megson; Project administration: Angie Elwin, Neil D'Cruze, Shubhobroto Ghosh; Funding acquisition: Neil D'Cruze, Shubhobroto Ghosh. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Neil D’Cruze https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8006-3084
Angie Elwin https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8583-3295
Emma Coulthard https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8935-9092
David Megson https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8881-3860
Vasudha Mishra https://orcid.org/0009-0008-5572-7473
Tiasa Adhya https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6478-9640
Aditya Banerjee https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7710-3583
Lauren Harrington https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7212-2336
All of the data that support the findings of this study are available in the main text or Supplementary Information.
Questionnaire survey questions
Data type: docx
Species list
Data type: xlsx
Price data
Data type: xlsx